Abstract
Numerosity discrimination, the ability to distinguish between sets with more and less items, is recognised as the foundation for higher numerical abilities. Understanding numerosity discrimination from a comparative perspective is hence pivotal in tracing the evolution of numerical representation systems. However, numerosity discrimination has been well studied only in vertebrates, where two innate systems of number representation have been described: an ‘analog magnitude system’ used to discriminate among numerosities by representing them as cardinal magnitudes and a ‘parallel individualisation system’ that allows precise discrimination among small arrays of items (≤4) by representing objects individually. We investigated the existence of quantity discrimination in an insect species (Tenebrio molitor) by using a spontaneous two-choice procedure in which males were exposed to substrates bearing odours from different numbers of females (≤4) in increasing numerosity ratios (1:4, 1:3 and 1:2). We show that males can discriminate sources of odours reflecting 1 versus 4 and 1 versus 3 females, but not 2 versus 4 or 1 versus 2, indicating that T. molitor males exhibit a marked preference for sources reflecting more female donors only when numerosity ratios are below 1:2. We discuss the functional significance of this finding and whether our pattern of results could be best explained by summation of a non-numerical continuous variable or by the existence of a numerosity discrimination mechanism with an operational signature ratio of 1:2.


Similar content being viewed by others
References
Agrillo C, Dadda M (2007) Discrimination of the larger shoal in the poeciliid fish Girardinus falcatus. Ethol Ecol & Evol 19:145–157
Agrillo C, Dadda M, Bisazza A (2007) Quantity discrimination in female mosquitofish. Anim Cogn 10:63–70
Barner D, Wood J, Hauser M, Carey S (2008) Evidence for a non-linguistic distinction between singular and plural sets in rhesus monkeys. Cognition 107:603–622
Beauchamp G (2003) Group-size effects on vigilance: a search for mechanisms. Behav Proc 63:111–121
Bekoff M, Allen C, Burghardt GM (2002) The cognitive animal: empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition. The MIT Press, Cambridge
Bhattacharya AK, Ameel JJ, Waldbauer GP (1970) A method for sexing living pupal and adult yellow mealworms. Ann Entomol Soc Am 63:1783
Boisvert MJ, Sherry DF (2006) Interval timing by an invertebrate, the bumble bee Bombus impatiens. Curr Biol 16:1636–1640
Brannon EM, Terrace HS (2002) The evolution and ontogeny of ordinal numerical ability. In: Bekoff M, Allen C, Burghardt GM (eds) The cognitive animal: empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 197–204
Brown WD (1990) Size-assortative mating in the blister beetle Lytta magister (Coleoptera: meloidae) is due to male and female preference for larger mates. Anim Behav 40:901–909
Buckingham JN, Wong BBM, Rosenthal GG (2007) Shoaling decisions in female swordtails: how do fish gauge group size? Behaviour 144:1333–1346
Butterworth B (1999) The mathematical brain. Macmillan, London
Carazo P, Sanchez E, Font E, Desfilis E (2004) Chemosensory cues allow male Tenebrio molitor beetles to asses the reproductive status of potential mates. Anim Behav 68:123–129
Carazo P, Font E, Alfthan B (2007) Chemical assessment of sperm competition levels and the evolution of internal spermatophore guarding. Proc R Soc B 274:261–267
Chen L, Zhang S, Srinivasan MV (2003) Global perception in small brains: topological pattern recognition in honey bees. PNAS 100:6884–6889
Chittka L, Brockmann A (2005) Perception space—the final frontier. PLoS Biol 3:0564–0568
Chittka L, Geiger K (1995) Can honey bees count landmarks? Anim Behav 49:159–164
Cleveland WS (1993) Visualizing data. Hobart Press, Summit
D’Ettorre P, Heinze J (2005) Individual recognition in ant queens. Curr Biol 15:2170–2174
Dacke M, Srinivasan MV (2008) Evidence for counting in insects. Anim Cogn 11:683–689
Dehaene S (1997) The number sense: how the mind creates mathematics. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Dehaene S, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Cohen L (1998) Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain. TINS 21:355–361
Drnevich JM (2003) Number of mating males and mating interval affect last-male sperm precedence in Tenebrio molitor L. Anim Behav 66:349–357
Drnevich JM, Hayes EF, Rutowski RL (2000) Sperm precedence, mating interval and a novel mechanism of paternity bias in a beetle (Tenebrio molitor L.). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48:447–451
Farnsworth GL, Smolinski JL (2006) Numerical discrimination by wild northern mockingbirds. Condor 108:953–957
Feigenson L, Dehaene S, Spelke E (2004) Core systems of number. TICS 8:307–314
Ferkin MH, Pierce AA, Sealand RO, delBarco-Trillo J (2005) Meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus, can distinguish more over-marks from fewer over-marks. Anim Cogn 8:182–189
Franks NR, Dornhaus A, Metherell BG, Nelson TR, Lanfear SA, Symes WS (2006) Not everything that counts can be counted: ants use multiple metrics for a single nest trait. Proc R Soc B 273:165–169
Gage MJG, Baker RR (1991) Ejaculate size varies with socio-sexual situation in an insect. Ecol Entomol 16:331–337
Gallistel CR, Gelman R (2000) Non-verbal numerical cognition: from the reals to the integers. TICS 4:59–65
Griffith OL (2001) The effect of mating on the pheromone system of the yellow mealworm beetle, Tenebrio molitor. Honours Thesis, University of Winnipeg
Happ GM (1969) Multiple sex pheromones of the mealworm beetle, Tenebrio molitor L. Nature 222:180–181
Happ GM, Wheeler J (1969) Bioassay, preliminary purification and effect of age, crowding and mating in the release of sex pheromone by female Tenebrio molitor. Ann Entomol Soc Am 62:846–851
Hauser M (2000) What do animals think about numbers? Am Sci 88:76–83
Hauser MD, Spelke E (2004) Evolutionary and developmental foundations of human knowledge: a case study of mathematics. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) The cognitive neurosciences III. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 853–864
Hauser MD, Carey S, Hauser LB (2000) Spontaneous number representation in semi-free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Proc R Soc Lond B 267:829–833
Hauser MD, Tsao F, Garcia P, Spelke E (2003) Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representation of numerical magnitudes by cotton-top tamarins. Proc R Soc Lond B 270:1441–1446
Holm S (1979) A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Statist 6:65–70
Honig WK, Stewart KE (1989) Discrimination of relative numerosity by pigeons. Anim Learn Behav 17:134–146
Karban R, Black CA, Weinbaum SA (2000) How 17-year cicadas keep track of time. Ecol Let 3:253–256
Lipton JS, Spelke ES (2003) Origins of number sense: large-number discrimination in human infants. Psych Sci 14:396–401
Lyon BE (2003) Egg recognition and counting reduce costs of avian conspecific brood parasitism. Nature 422:495–499
McComb K, Packer C, Pusey A (1994) Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo. Anim Behav 47:379–387
Meck WH, Church RM (1983) A mode control model of counting and timing processes. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 9:320–334
Meck WH, Church RM, Gibbon J (1985) Temporal integration in duration and number discrimination. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 11:591–597
Menzel R, Brembs B, Giurfa M (2007) Cognition in invertebrates. In: Strausfeld NJ, Bullock TH (eds) Evolution of nervous systems in invertebrates, vol 11. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 403–442
Moyer RS, Landauer TK (1967) Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature 215:1519–1520
Pepperberg IM (2006) Grey parrot numerical competence: a review. Anim Cogn 9:377–391
Rantala MJ, Kortet R, Kotiaho JS, Vainikka A, Suhonen J (2003) Condition dependence of pheromones and immune function in the grain beetle Tenebrio molitor. Func Ecol 17:534–540
Roberts W (1997) Principles of animal cognition. McGraw-Hill, New York
Rugani R, Regolin L, Vallortigara G (2007) Rudimental numerical competence in 5-day-old domestic chicks (Gallus gallus): identification of ordinal position. J Exp Psych 33:21–31
Shettleworth SJ (1998) Cognition, evolution and behaviour. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Siegel S, Castellan NJJ (1989) Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. McGraw-Hill, New York
Skorupski P, Chittka L (2006) Animal cognition: an insect’s sense of time? Curr Biol 16:1636–1640
Tanaka Y, Honda H, Ohsawa K, Yamamoto I (1986) A sex attractant of the yellow mealworm, Tenebrio molitor L. and its role in the mating behavior. J Pesticide Sci 11:49–55
Tegeder RW, Krause J (1995) Density dependence and numerosity in fright stimulated aggregation behaviour of shoaling fish. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 350:381–390
Thom MD, Hurst JL (2004) Individual recognition by scent. Ann Zool Fennici 41:765–787
Thomas ML, Simmons LW (2009) Male-derived cuticular hydrocarbons signal sperm competition intensity and affect ejaculate expenditure in crickets. Proc R Soc B 276:383–388
Trick LM, Pylyshyn ZW (1994) Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychol Rev 101:80–102
Tschinkel W, Willson C, Bern HA (1967) Sex pheromone of the mealworm beetle (Tenebrio molitor). J Exp Zool 164:81–86
Uller C, Jaeger R, Guidry G, Martin C (2003) Salamanders (Plethodon cinereus) go for more: rudiments of number in an amphibian. Anim Cogn 6:105–112
Wittlinger M, Wehner R, Wolf H (2006) The ant odometer: stepping on stilts and stumps. Science 312:1965–1967
Worden BD, Parker PG, Pappas PW (2000) Parasites reduce attractiveness and reproductive success in male grain beetles. Anim Behav 59:543–550
Wyatt TD (2003) Pheromones and animal behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Wynn K (1998) Psychological foundations of number: numerical competence in human infants. TICS 2:296–302
Xu F (2003) Numerosity discrimination in infants: evidence for two systems of representations. Cognition 89:B15–B25
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to M.D. Hauser, R. Menzel and three anonymous referees for their insightful criticisms and comments on a previous version of this manuscript. We also wish to thank Carlos Sampedro for his help in the maintenance of insect cultures. P.C. was supported by a research grant (FPU) from the Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia of Spain.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Carazo, P., Font, E., Forteza-Behrendt, E. et al. Quantity discrimination in Tenebrio molitor: evidence of numerosity discrimination in an invertebrate?. Anim Cogn 12, 463–470 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-008-0207-7
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-008-0207-7