Abstract
Although the technical problem-solving expertise of nonhuman primates has been investigated extensively in captivity, few species have been tested in their natural habitats. Here I examine the physical cognition of wild savanna baboons (Papio anubis), a species that occupies an omnivorous foraging niche in which a variety of embedded food items are extracted and processed. Baboons were tested on three puzzles, each involving high-quality food that required removal from a novel obstruction: (1) a string-pulling puzzle in which food was hung from tree branches, (2) a twig-dipping puzzle in which food was embedded in a vertical tube, and (3) a stick-pushing puzzle in which food was contained in a horizontal conduit. The baboons failed to solve the second and third puzzles even when tools had been appropriately positioned in advance. And although they solved the first puzzle, their actions (running while holding food that was still attached to the string), suggested they did not fully comprehend the string’s connective property. The baboons’ performance might reflect the time constraints of life in the wild, which relative to captivity may provide fewer opportunities for the development of understanding about the physical properties of objects and their potential uses as tools. Further experiments on the physical cognition of baboons and many other primate species in their natural habitats would help test this ontogenetic hypothesis. Such field experiments would be especially fruitful if they continued to target extractive foragers like baboons: these experiments could simultaneously provide a test of phylogenetic hypotheses that invoke extractive foraging as the key stimulus for brain expansion in savanna-dwelling hominids.

Similar content being viewed by others
References
Altmann SA (1998) Foraging for survival: yearling baboons in Africa. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Altmann SA, Altmann J (1970) Baboon ecology: African field research. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Beck BB (1972) Tool use in captive hamadryas baboons. Primates 13:276–296
Beck BB (1973) Cooperative tool use by captive hamadryas baboons. Science 182:594–597
Beck BB (1980) Animal tool behavior: the use and manufacture of tools by animals. Garland STPM Press, New York
Benhar EE, Samuel D (1978) A case of tool use in captive olive baboons (Papio anubis). Primates 19:385–389
Birch H (1945) The relation of previous experience to insightful problem solving. J Comp Psychol 38:367–383
Biro D, Inoue-Nakamura N, Tonooka R, Yamakoshi G, Sousa C, Matsuzawa T (2003) Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments. Anim Cogn 6:213–223
Bolwig N (1961) An intelligent tool-using baboon. S Afr J Sci 57:147–152
Byrne R (1995) The thinking ape: evolutionary origins of intelligence. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Byrne RW, Whiten A (ed) (1988) Machiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Clarendon Press, Oxford
Byrne RW (2000) How monkeys find their way: leadership, coordination, and cognitive maps of African baboons. In: Boinski S, Garber PA (eds) On the move: how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 491–518
Flombaum JI, Junge JA, Hauser MD (2005) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers. Cognition 97:315–325
Engh AL, Hoffmeier RR, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2006) Who, me? Can baboons infer the target of vocalizations? Anim Behav 71:381–387
Gould JL, Gould CG (1994) The animal mind. Scientific American Library, New York
Halsey LG, Bezerra BM, Souto AS (2006) Can wild common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) solve the parallel strings task? Anim Cogn 9:229–233
Hamilton WJ, Buskirk RE, Buskirk WH (1975) Defensive stoning by baboons. Nature 256:488–489
Hare B (2007) From nonhuman to human mind—what changed and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science 16:60–64
Hauser M, Spaulding B (2006) Wild rhesus monkeys generate causal inferences about possible and impossible physical transformations in the absence of experience. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:7181–7185
Hauser MD, MacNeilage P, Ware M (1996) Numerical representations in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:1514–1517
Hauser MD, Carey S, Hauser LB (2000) Spontaneous number representation in semi-free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 267:829–833
Hayashi M (2007) Stacking of blocks by chimpanzees: developmental processes and physical understanding. Anim Cogn 10:89–103
Heinrich B (1995) An experimental investigation of insight in the common raven Corvus corax. Auk 112:994–1003
Heinrich B, Bugnyar T (2005) Testing problem solving in ravens: string-pulling to reach food. Ethology 111:962–976
Huber L, Gajdon GK (2006) Technical intelligence in animals: the kea model. Anim Cogn 9:295–305
Inoue-Nakamura N, Matsuzawa T (1997) Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psych 111:159–173
Köhler W (1927) The mentality of apes. Viking, New York
Kummer H (1995) In quest of the sacred baboon: a scientist’s journey. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ
Laidre ME (2007) Do captive mandrills invent new gestures? Anim Cogn (in press)
Maple T (1975) Aggressive object displays of captive baboons. J Mammol 56:949–950
Matsuzawa T (1994) Field experiments on use of stone tools by chimpanzees in the wild. In: Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne PG (eds) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 351–370
Matsuzawa T (1996) Chimpanzee intelligence in nature and in captivity: isomorphism of symbol use and tool use. In: McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T (eds) Great ape societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 196–209
Matsuzawa T (1999) Communication and tool use in chimpanzees: cultural and social contexts. In: Hauser MD, Konishi M (eds) The design of animal communication. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 645–671
Mulcahy NJ, Call J (2006) How great apes perform on a modified trap-tube task. Anim Cogn 9: 193–199
Muruthi P (1997) Socioecological correlates of parental care and demography in savanna baboons. Ph.D. thesis, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ
Oyen OJ (1978) Stone-eating and tool-use among olive baboons. Texas J Sci 30:295
Parker ST, Gibson KR (1979) A developmental model for the evolution of language and intelligence in early hominids. Behav Brain Sci 2:367–408
Pettet A (1975) Matters arising—defensive stoning by baboons. Nature 258:549
Pickford M (1975) Matters arising—defensive stoning by baboons. Nature 258:549–550
Povinelli DJ (2000) Folk physics for apes: the chimpanzee’s theory of how the world works. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Sakura O, Matsuzawa T (1991) Flexibility of wild chimpanzee nut-cracking behavior using stone hammers and anvils: an experimental analysis. Ethology 87:237–248
Santos LR, Rosati A, Sproul C, Spaulding B, Hauser MD (2005) Means-means-end tool choice in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): finding the limits on primates’ knowledge of tools. Anim Cogn 8:236–246
Santos LR, Pearson HM, Spaepen GM, Tsao F, Hauser MD (2006) Probing the limits of tool competence: experiments with two non-tool-using species (Cercopithecus aethiops and Saguinus oedipus). Anim Cogn 9:94–109
Schiller PH (1957) Innate motor action as a basis of learning. In: Schiller CH (ed) Instinctive behavior. International Universities Press, New York, pp 264–287
Sugiyama Y, Fushimi T, Sakura O, Matsuzawa T (1993) Hand preference and tool use in wild chimpanzees. Primates 34:151–159
Sulkowski GM, Hauser MD (2001) Can rhesus monkeys spontaneously subtract? Cognition 79:239–262
Tebbich S, Seed AM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2007) Non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus, solve the trap-tube problem. Anim Cogn 10:225–231
Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate cognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford
van Lawick-Goodall J, van Lawick H, Packer C (1973) Tool-use in free-living baboons in the Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Nature 241:212–213
Visalberghi E, Limongelli L (1994) Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J Comp Psychol 108:15–22
Vlasak AN (2006) Global and local spatial landmarks: their role during foraging by Columbian ground squirrels (Spermophilus columbianus). Anim Cogn 9:71–80
Westergaard GC (1999) Structural analysis of tool-use by tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 2:141–145
Acknowledgments
I thank Raphael Eiyanae Erengai for assistance and genial companionship in the field, and the Mpala Research Centre for allowing me to carry out this study, especially Nick Georgiadis and Chris Odhiambo. I am indebted to Sango Leleshu for making the string-pulling experiment a reality, Christie Riehl for serendipitously supplying the string, Philip Muruthi for expert discussion during a visit to the field, and Dan Rubenstein for loaning video equipment and binoculars. Stuart Altmann, Tatiana Czeschlik, Jonathan Flombaum, Jim Gould, Robert Seyfarth, and the anonymous referees provided helpful comments on the manuscript. The experiments comply with the current laws of the country (Kenya) in which they were performed. Supported by a grant from the Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology and an NSF Graduate Research Fellowship.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
ESM1 (WMV 3998 kb)
ESM2 (WMV 3733 kb)
ESM3 (WMV 4514 kb)
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Laidre, M.E. Spontaneous performance of wild baboons on three novel food-access puzzles. Anim Cogn 11, 223–230 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0104-5
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0104-5