Skip to main content

Social Cognition and the Pragmatics of Ideology

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Pragmemes and Theories of Language Use

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology ((PEPRPHPS,volume 9))

  • 988 Accesses

Abstract

In this paper a theory of ideology which is based on the interaction of social and cognitive factors is discussed. This theory takes a pragmatic approach to build its main background theory. The present model would also provide evidence for the need to expand our understanding of the basic units of pragmatic analysis and pragmatic acts in general, in order to include the cognitive and social factors associated with language and ideology, as implied by the general theory of the pragmeme.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    We are identifying a model of a cognitive system with a mathematical dynamical system instantiated by it (Giunti 1997). The dynamic system is a proper model of the cognitive system if it captures or describes one or more of its aspects. The cognitive aspect of a cognitive system that a simulation model describes is the process or behavioral pattern associated with the completion of a task or environmental change. For example, if the behavior consists in the solution of a logical problem, the cognitive model will simulate the subjects problem-solving processes (Newell and Simon 1972). If the behavior to be modeled is a linguistic acquisition process, for example how a child acquires the English past tense system, the cognitive model will attempt to simulate this process. The simulation model can be symbolic or connectionistic (Rumelhart and McClelland 1986).

  2. 2.

    The emergence of the group of theories covered under the ‘Optimality Theory’ umbrella can be viewed as a compromise between higher level symbolic theories of linguistic and cognitive processes and lower level neural connectionistic networks (Prince and Smolensky 1997).

References

  • Alexander, J., & Smith, P. (1993). The discourse of American Civil Society: A new proposal for cultural studies. Theory and Society, 22, 151–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capone, A. (2005). Pragmemes. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 1355–1371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, W. (2001). Malaysias resilient pseudodemocracy. Journal of Democracy, 12, 43–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collier, D., & Adcock, R. (1999). Democracy and dichotomies. A pragmatic approach to choices about concepts. Annual Review of Political Science, 2, 537–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collier, D., & Levitsky, S. (1997). Democracy with adjectives: Conceptual innovation in comparative research. World Politics, 49, 430–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collier, D., & Mahon, J. (1993). Conceptual stretching revisited: Adapting categories in comparative analysis. American Political Science Review, 87, 845–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conte, R., & Castelfranchi, C. (1995). Cognitive and social action. London: UCL Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, R. A. (1989). Democracy and its critics. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, L. (1999). Developing democracy: Toward consolidation. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Entrikin, N. (1991). The betweenness of place: Towards a geography of modernity. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedom House (2000/1989–1997). Country ratings: Annual survey of freedom country scores.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnometodology. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gee, J. P. (1992). The social mind: Language, ideology, and social practice. New York: Bergin & Garvey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giunti, M. (1997). Computation, dynamics and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldblatt, D. (1997). Democracy in Europe: 1939–1938. In D. Potter, D. Goldblatt, M. Kiloh, & P. Lewis (Eds.), Democratization (pp. 95–117). Malden: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, P. (1989). Essays in the ways of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobbs, J. (1990). Artificial intelligence and collective intentionality. In P. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communication (pp. 445–459). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huhns, M., & Singh, M. (Eds.). (1998). Readings in agents. San Francisco: Morgan Kaufmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jakobson, R. (1960). Closing statement: Linguistics and poetics. In T. A. Sebeok (Ed.), Style in language. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamp, H., & Reyle, U. (1993). From discourse to logic. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1996). Moral politics: What conservatives know that liberals dont. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1999). Metaphor, morality, and politics: Or, why conservatives have left liberals in the dust. In R. Wheeler (Ed.), The workings of language (pp. 139–155). Westport: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luhmann, N. (1982). The differentiation of society. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luhmann, N. (1989). Ecological communication. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mey, J. (2001). Pragmatics. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince, & Smolensky. (1993). Optimality theory. Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince, & Smolensky. (1997). Optimality: From neural networks to universal grammar. Science, 275, 1604–1610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ricoeur, P. (1984). Time and narrative. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D., & McClelland, J. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In D. Rumelhart & J. McClelland (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing (Vol. 2, pp. 216–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartori, G. (1987). The theory of democracy revisited. New Jersey: Chatham House Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schatz, S., & Gutiérrez-Rexach, J. (2004). Conceptual structure and social change. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E., & Sacks, H. (1979). Opening up closing. In R. Turner (Ed.), Ethnometodology: Selected Readings (pp. 233–264). Baltimore: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1990). Collective intentions and actions. In P. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communication (pp. 401–415). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, C., & Weaver, W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimony, A. (1995). Cybernetics and social entities. In K. Gavroglu (Ed.), Science, politics and social practices (pp. 181–196). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sokal, R. R. (1975). Classification: Purposes, principles, progress, prospects. Science, 185, 115–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stalnaker, R. (1984). Inquiry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starr, P. (1992). Social categories and claims in the liberal state. Social Research, 59, 263–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toumela, R. (1989). Collective action, supervenience, and constitution. Synthese, 80, 243–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toumela, R. (1991). We will do it: An analysis of group-intentions. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 11, 249–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toumela, R., & Miller, K. (1988). We-intentions. Philosophical Studies, 53, 367–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turing, A. (1950). Computing machines and intelligence. Mind, 49, 433–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turk, A. T. (1982). Political criminality: The defiance and defense of authority. Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, V. (1974). Dramas, fields and metaphors. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. (2001). Cognitive dimensions of social science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Benthem, J. (1996). Exploring logical dynamics. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, G. (1999). Multiagent systems. A modern approach to distributed artificial intelligence. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zerubavel, E. (1997). Social mindscapes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Javier Gutiérrez-Rexach .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gutiérrez-Rexach, J., Schatz, S. (2016). Social Cognition and the Pragmatics of Ideology. In: Allan, K., Capone, A., Kecskes, I. (eds) Pragmemes and Theories of Language Use. Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology, vol 9. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-43491-9_32

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-43491-9_32

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-43490-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-43491-9

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics