Abstract
The ability to act on information flexibly is one of the cornerstones of intelligent behavior. As particularly informative example, tool-oriented behavior has been investigated to determine to which extent nonhuman animals understand means–end relations, object affordances, and have specific motor skills. Even planning with foresight, goal-directed problem solving and immediate causal inference have been a focus of research. However, these cognitive abilities may not be restricted to tool-using animals but may be found also in animals that show high levels of curiosity, object exploration and manipulation, and extractive foraging behavior. The kea, a New Zealand parrot, is a particularly good example. We here review findings from laboratory experiments and field observations of keas revealing surprising cognitive capacities in the physical ___domain. In an experiment with captive keas, the success rate of individuals that were allowed to observe a trained conspecific was significantly higher than that of naive control subjects due to their acquisition of some functional understanding of the task through observation. In a further experiment using the string-pulling task, a well-probed test for means–end comprehension, we found the keas finding an immediate solution that could not be improved upon in nine further trials. We interpreted their performance as insightful in the sense of being sensitive of the relevant functional properties of the task and thereby producing a new adaptive response without trial-and-error learning. Together, these findings contribute to the ongoing debate on the distribution of higher cognitive skills in the animal kingdom by showing high levels of sensorimotor intelligence in animals that do not use tools. In conclusion, we suggest that the ‘Technical intelligence hypothesis’ (Byrne, Machiavellian intelligence II: extensions and evaluations, pp 289–211, 1997), which has been proposed to explain the origin of the ape/monkey grade-shift in intelligence by a selection pressure upon an increased efficiency in foraging behavior, should be extended, that is, applied to some birds as well.

Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
A direct comparison is not possible because of the lack of first trial measurements of the performance of the Grey parrots. Pepperberg (2004) only reported that two birds immediately performed and correctly repeated the targeted action each time without any hesitation, although the actions were not necessarily performed smoothly.
Despite a number of caveats (see, for discussion, Doré and Dumas 1987; Parker and McKinney 1999; Pepperberg 1999, 2002) and criticism on the basis of two principal grounds, recapitulationism and anthropomorphism (Vauclair 1996), Piaget's theory and its application to animal cognition has many strengths and is well suited to address the general issue of the origin, nature, ontogeny, and function of animal as well as human knowledge (Doré and Dumas 1987; Vauclair 1999).
References
Antinucci F (1989) Systematic comparison of early sensorimotor development. In: Antinucci F (ed) Cognitive structure and development in nonhuman primates. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hilldsale, NJ, pp 67–85
Beck BB (1980) Animal tool behavior. Garland Press, New York
Bugnyar T, Heinrich B (2006) Pilfering ravens, Corvus corax, adjust their behaviour to social context and identity of competitors. Anim Cogn DOI 10.1007/s10071-006-0035-6
Bugnyar T, Kotrschal K (2002) Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in ravens, Corvus corax: is it tactical deception? Anim Behav 64:185–195
Byrne RW (1997) The technical intelligence hypothesis: an additional evolutionary stimulus to intelligence? In: Whiten A, Byrne RW (eds) Machiavellian intelligence. vol II: extensions and evaluations. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 289–211
Byrne RW (1998) Comment on Boesch and Tomasello's “chimpanzee and human cultures”. Curr Anthropol 39:604–605
Byrne RW, Whiten A (1988) Machiavellian intelligence. Social expertise and the evolution of the intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Clarendon Press, Oxford
Call J, Tomasello M (1996) The effect of humans on the cognitive development of apes. In: Russon AE, Bard KA, Parker ST (eds) Reaching into thought: the minds of the great apes. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 371–403
Chevalier-Skolnikoff S (1989) Spontaneous tool use and sensorimotor intelligence in Cebus compared with other monkeys and apes. Behav Brain Sci 12:561–588
Clayton DA (1978) Socially facilitated behavior. Q Rev Biol 53:373–392
Diamond J, Bond AB (1999) Kea, bird of paradox. The evolution and behavior of a New Zealand parrot. University of California Press, Berkeley
Doré FY, Dumas C (1987) Psychology of animal cognition: Piagetian studies. Psychol Bull 102:219–233
Dücker G, Rensch B (1977) The solution of patterned string problems by birds. Behavior 62:164–173
Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2004) The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science 306:1903–1907
Funk MS (2002) Problem solving skills in young yellow-crowned parakeets (Cyanoramphus auriceps). Anim Cogn 5:167–176
Gajdon G, Fijn N, Huber L (2004) Testing social learning in a wild mountain parrot, the kea (Nestor notabilis). Learn Behav 32:62–71
Gajdon GK, Fijn N, Huber L (2006) Limited spread of innovation in a wild parrot, the kea (Nestor notabilis). Anim Cogn 9:173–181. DOI: 10.1007/s10071–006-0018–7
Hauser MD (1997) Artifactual kinds and functional design features: what a primate understands without language. Cognition 64:285–308
Hauser MD (2000) Wild minds: what animals really think. Holt, New York
Heinrich B (1995) An experimental investigation of insight in common ravens (Corvus corax). Auk 112:994–1003
Heinrich B (2000) Testing insight in ravens. In: Heyes C, Huber L (eds) The evolution of cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 289–305
Heinrich B, Bugnyar T (2005) Testing problem solving in ravens: string-pulling to reach food. Ethology 111: 962–976
Heyes CM (1998) Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behav Brain Sci 21:101–148
Hood BM, Hauser MD, Anderson L, Santos L (1999) Gravity biases in a non-human primate? Dev Sci 2:35–41
Huber L (1998) Movement imitation as faithful copying in the absence of insight. Behav Brain Sci 22:694
Huber L (in press) Emulation learning: the integration of technical and social cognition. In: Dautenhahn K, Nehaniv CL (eds) Models and mechanisms of imitation and social learning in robots, humans and animals: behavioural, social and communicative dimensions. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK
Huber L, Rechberger S, Taborsky M (2001) Social learning affects object exploration and manipulation in keas, Nestor notabilis. Anim Behav 62:945–954
Humphrey NK (1976) The social function of intellect. In: Bateson PPG, Hinde RA (eds) Growing points in ethology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 303–317
Hunt GR, Rutledge RB, Gray RD (2006) The right tool for the job: what strategy do wild New Caledonian crows use? Anim Cogn DOI 10.1007/s10071-006-0047-2
Iwaniuk AN, Dean KM, Nelson JE (2005) Interspecific allometry of the brain and brain regions in parrots (Psittaciformes): comparisons with other birds and primates. Brain Behav Evol 65:40–59
Johnston R (1999) The kea (Nestor notabilis): A New Zealand problem or problem solver? Doctoral thesis, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand
Jolly A (1966) Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence. Science 153:501–506
Köhler W (1921) The mentality of apes. Methuen, London
Kummer H (1995) Causal knowledge in animals. In: Sperber D, Premack D, Premack AJ (eds) Causal cognition: a multidisciplinary debate. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 26–39
Lefebvre L (2000) Feeding innovations and their cultural transmission in bird populations. In: Heyes CM, Huber L (eds) The evolution of cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
Lefebvre L, Reader SM, Sol D (2004) Brains, innovations and evolution in birds and primates. Brain Behav Evol 63:233–246
Marler P (1996) Are primates smarter than birds? Curr Ornithol 13:1–32
Mendes N, Huber L (2004) Object permanence in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). J Comp Psychol 118:103–112
Osthaus B, Lea SEG, Slater AM (2005) Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) fail to show understanding of means-end connections in a string-pulling task. Anim Cogn 8: 37–47
Parker ST, Gibson KR (1977) Object manipulation, tool use, and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in cebus monkeys and great apes. J Hum Evol 6:623–641
Parker ST, McKinney ML (1999) Origins of intelligence. The evolution of cognitive development in monkeys, apes, and humans. The Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, London
Pepperberg IM (1999) The Alex studies: cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrots. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
Pepperberg IM (2002) The value of the Piagetian framework for comparative cognitive studies. Anim Cogn 5:177–182
Pepperberg IM (2004) “Insightful” string-pulling in Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) is affected by vocal competence. Anim Cogn 7:263–266
Pepperberg IM (2006) Grey parrot numerical competence: a review. Anim Cogn DOI 10.1007/s10071-006-0034-7
Pepperberg IM, Willner MR, Gravitz LB (1997) Development of Piagetian object permanence in a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). J Comp Psychol 111:63–75
Piaget J (1936) La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant. Neuenburg, Paris
Povinelli DJ (2000) Folk physics for apes. The chimpanzee's theory of how the world works. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Rumbaugh DM, Beran MJ, Hillix WA (2000) Cause-effect reasoning in humans and animals. In: Heyes C, Huber L (eds) The evolution of cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 221–238
Santos L, Ericson BN, Hauser MD (1999) Constraints on problem solving and inhibition: object retrieval in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus oedipus). J Comp Psychol 113:186–193
Seibt U, Wickler W (2006) Individuality in problem solving: string pulling in two carduelis species (Aves: passeriformes). Ethology 112:493–502
Spelke E, Breinlinger K, Macomber J, Jacobson K (1992) Origins of knowledge. Psychol Rev 99:605–612
Sperber D, Premack D, Premack AJ (1995) Causal cognition: a multidisciplinary debate. Clarendon Press, Oxford
Tebbich S, Taborsky M, Winkler H (1996) Social manipulation causes cooperation in keas. Anim Behav 52:1–10
The Avian Brain Nomenclature Consortium (2005) Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:151–159
Thorpe WH (1956) Learning and instinct in animals. Methuen, London
Tomasello M (1996) Do apes ape? In: Galef B, Heyes C (eds) Social learning in animals: The roots of culture. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 319–346
Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate cognition. Oxford University Press, New York
Tomasello M, Davis-Dasilva M, Camak L, Bard K (1987) Observational learning of tool-use by young chimpanzees. Hum Evol 2:175–183
Tomasello M, Kruger AC, Ratner HH (1993) Cultural learning. Behav Brain Sci 16:495–552
Vauclair J (1996) Animal cognition: an introduction to modern comparative psychology. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, London
Vince MA (1961) “String-pulling” in birds. III: The successful response in greenfinches and canaries. Behavior 17:103–129
Visalberghi E, Tomasello M (1998) Primate causal understanding in the physical and psychological domains. Behav Process 42:189–203
Voelkl B, Huber L (2000) True imitation in marmosets. Anim Behav 60:195–202
Want SC, Harris PL (2002) How do children ape? Applying concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental study of ‘imitation’ in children. Dev Sci 5:1–41
Watanabe S, Huber L (2006) Animal logics: decisions in the absence of human language. Anim Cogn DOI 10.1007/s10071-006-0043-6
Weir AAS, Kacelnik A (2006) New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) creatively re-design tools by bending or unbending metal strips according to needs. Anim Cogn DOI 10.1007/s10071-006-0052-5
Weir AS, Chappell J, Kacelnik A (2002) Shaping of hooks in New Caledonian crows. Science 297:981
Werdenich D, Huber L (2006) A case of quick problem solving in birds: string-pulling in keas (Nestor notabilis). Anim Behav 71:855–863
Whiten A, Custance DM, Gomez J-C, Teixidor P, Bard KA (1996) Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 110:3–14
Whiten A, Horner V, Litchfield CA, Marshall-Pescini S (2004) How do apes ape? Learn Behav 32:36–52
Willatts P (1999) Development of means-end behavior in young infants: pulling a support to retrieve a distant object. Dev Psychol 35:651–667
Acknowledgements
We are grateful for the assistance and support of the Department of Conservation of New Zealand. Financial support came from the Austrian Science Fund (BIO P15027). We thank two anonymous referees for valuable comments on an earlier draft of the paper.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
This contribution is part of the special issue “Animal Logics” (Watanabe and Huber 2006).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Huber, L., Gajdon, G. Technical intelligence in animals: the kea model. Anim Cogn 9, 295–305 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-006-0033-8
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-006-0033-8